New Guidelines on How to Accurately Convey ADHD Information

26
2568

When it comes to information on ADHD, there are no rules on what are considered good and bad practices. To this day, this results in websites, news messages and even academic textbooks that are very unbalanced and filled with fallacies.

Unbalanced, for instance, in the sense that much emphasis is placed on brain and genetic studies that to this day have cost billions of dollars, while showing only very small associations—not providing any basis for biological screening. At the same time, correlations that are much stronger often go unmentioned. A vivid example is that healthcare professionals tend do classify the youngest in a classroom as having ADHD up to twice as often as their older classmates—due to their normal age-appropriate behavior: a stunning but long-known research finding that has insufficiently found its way to a broader audience. Fallacies are also very common, an important one being the ‘nominal fallacy’: thinking that by naming behaviors using denominators like ADHD and autism, we have explained them.

Now there is a new set of guidelines (available here) on how to talk about ADHD. The guidelines are meant for mental health professionals and journalists who write about ADHD, clinicians who explain ADHD to their clients and their families, and those who have the experiences that have been labeled as ADHD. The guidelines are meant to help explain what research on ADHD has actually found, its strengths and limitations, and more accurate ways to express what the classification means.

A sweet girl schoolgirl on a gray background. Works homework by reading a book in a yellow cover.

The creation of the guidelines began in 2018, when the Dutch research collective ‘Druk & Dwars’ (Wild and Willful) initiated a taskforce for their development. The project was funded by ZonMW, an independent government body that finances research in health, healthcare and well-being. I chaired the project, which was closely related to my research on reification. A variety of healthcare professionals participated, including professors in pedagogy, psychiatry (neuro)psychology, policymakers in the healthcare domain, those experienced with ADHD, and many more.

The group did have a somewhat difficult start. Soon enough however, it became clear that none of the taskforce’s members denied the phenomenon of people feeling restless, having trouble concentrating, for whatever reason or by whatever causes. Most in the taskforce also agreed that the criteria for the ADHD construct to be used are highly subjective and contextually situated and there is a risk of pathologizing normal (or often functional) behavior. Additionally, the discourse is too biologically oriented and often insufficiently acknowledges the influence of environmental and societal factors.

Fortunately,  studies show that there are also many good practices available, explaining the outcomes of ADHD research very well. This led the group to choose a comparative approach, juxtaposing and comparing more and less desired examples to explain how and why one example is better and why the other examples may give a skewed/false perception of research outcomes.

Receiving a slow but positive reception in the Netherlands in 2022 and 2023, several additional practitioners in the field of psychiatry and psychology committed to translating the guidelines to English, resulting in the current revised and stylized version. Many topics are covered in the guidelines, mostly relating to causes and motives for the behaviors that fall under the ADHD umbrella. Topics include societal norms and contextual factors but also genetics, brain anatomy/physiology/chemistry, course over the lifetime, and more.

Although many subjects are discussed, some fallacies seem particularly important in perpetuating a flawed narrative that not only plagues ADHD, but many more of the classifications defined in the DSM. These fallacies are, first and foremost the ‘ecological fallacy’: the fact that average(!) group outcomes are used to justify categorically naming ADHD, a ‘neurobiological’ or even ‘neurological disorder’ while—by far—not every individual with ADHD has any neurobiological difference. This happens for instance in relation to anatomical brain studies—that indicate that those with ADHD slightly more often have a delayed brain maturation. Just as women have smaller brains than man on average, this is not true for every woman, and there are women with larger brains than the average man.

Furthermore, a smaller brain is not indicative of a brain condition. This issue, known in moral philosophy as the ‘fact-value’ distinction is also brought to the fore: differences (by whatever definition considered factual) are not disorders (as this is a statement of value, not of fact). The contemporary neurodiversity debate also relates to this subject and the guidelines provide concrete examples on how to avoid the trap of pathologizing what research can only show to be differences between people.

Genetic studies are also the cause of many misunderstandings. Mad in America printed an article on an earlier study on how many academic textbooks leave readers in the dark about the fact that genetic differences can hardly explain any of the ADHD behaviors. Those genetic variants that do contribute appear far more often in ‘normal’ people in absolute terms.

These and many other flaws in sources on ADHD are discussed in the guidelines. This gives everyone, especially website editors, journalists and informed parents an opportunity to improve information or talk back to sources that falsely suggest that those classified as having ADHD children and adults alike suffer from a brain condition in most cases. Other factors than dysfunctional brains, like schools being underfunded or our culture not being well-adapted to individual differences, are often overlooked. The predominantly biomedical narrative puts children at risk of being unnecessarily medicated and pathologized, can lead to despair and prognostic pessimism and hinders parents and teachers to make an informed decision to help their child or pupil.

This is, to our knowledge, the first set of guidelines on information about any of the disorders defined by the DSM. Many of the fallacies and imbalances also occur in relation to others classifications such as autism, depression and bipolar disorder—increasingly echoed by influencers on social media and resulting in an increasing burden on healthcare systems globally. Yet beyond the stigma and despair that individuals can experience due to the skewed and often misleading information, we have built a resource-spending institutional world that depends on the revenues based on the narrative of medically treatable individual disorders. As these institutions have little incentive for change, it is time governments step up and start investing in more research into the medicalized narrative and push for more market control and guidelines on psycho-education.

For questions or feedback on these guidelines, you can contact the editor and co-author dr. Sanne te Meerman, University of Groningen. [email protected]

***

Mad in America hosts blogs by a diverse group of writers. These posts are designed to serve as a public forum for a discussion—broadly speaking—of psychiatry and its treatments. The opinions expressed are the writers’ own.

26 COMMENTS

  1. I’m concerned about how often ADHD/Autism is (self)diagnosed and so very little is heard about FAS/FASD. In NZ we have high rates of bung drinking both male & female. Yet it’s almost impossible to get conversation going about the in utero effects of alcohol on the fetus

    Report comment

  2. “This happens for instance in relation to anatomical brain studies—that indicate that those with ADHD slightly more often have a delayed brain maturation. Just as women have smaller brains than man on average, this is not true for every woman, and there are women with larger brains than the average man.”

    At this point it seems reasonable to point out that the brain size difference between men and women is 10 [!] times as large as the difference between ADHD and non-ADHD. If the brain size difference would in some way explain ADHD, women would “all” have mega turbo hyper ADHD. Clearly they don’t.

    Report comment

  3. You can’t convey accurate PTSD information because there is no accurate PTSD information and there is no accurate social information. It’s all 100% bullshit. Proof is in the pudding. Next…

    (Good job I’m not the one marking your homework people.)

    Report comment

  4. Thank you, No-One.

    The article articulates that “not every individual with ADHD has any neurobiological difference”. ‘ADHD’, like the other 296 disorders in the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental ‘Disorders’, lacks valid scientific evidence, and therefore, does not exist (Breggin (1991), Burstow (2015), Whitaker (2010), Woolfolk (2001), and Szasz (1987).

    The text also contends that “it is time governments step up and start investing in more research”. What if governments acknowledged the flaws in the research they fund or receive funding from, and recognised that they are using the DSM to exert control over their citizens? Why does it seem that there is a lack of acknowledgment regarding the perception that psychiatry has operated as a fundamentally flawed discipline since its inception?

    Report comment

      • :-). Assuming that’s not just sarcasm, I’ll give my view. Much of the definitional power of the social sciences is expressed through discourse. Social science discursively defines what problems we should focus on, howto study them, and howto present the results. Currently, for many historical and cultural reasons, problem definitions are mostly individual, and the (institutionalized) solution space is extremely limited in its wake: our society is very much psychiatrized: https://www.frontiersin.org/journals/psychiatry/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.645556/full
        To deconstruct this undue power imbalance, we must definitely also deconstruct the discourse on which the power is based. These guidelines help do that. But they were funded too (to some small extent).

        Fortunately, governments can easily fund these types of studies, and save money in the proces. If we spend 10% of what we are now spending on biological studies into the DSM’s classification, there’d be plenty to go around. And we can skip a lot of what we are now spending on biological studies: the DSM’s ever expending umbrella classifications were never very useful for proper biomedical studies anyway (yes, now I’m being polite again 🙂 ).

        Report comment

        • Ah, but they rarely do fund this sort of study! The heavily moneyed biopsych advocates have a lot of sayso about what is studied, and protest when their pet drugs and companies get short shrift. Besides which, most studies theses days are funded by drug companies, not government entities. There’s little chance non-drug interventions will ever establish a sufficient “evidence base” to match those the drug companies can muster up!

          Report comment

      • The other day I got an invitation from Brain & Behavior Research Foundation for a half hour educational seminar on ADHD which would give you “a deeper understanding of ADHD , the different ways the disorder manifests, the differences in presentation in boys and girls” so that teachers, counselors and school staff can “make referrals for evaluation”.

        Report comment

  5. “genetic differences can hardly explain any of the ADHD behaviors.”

    I disagree. There is a high rate of MTHFR mutations in ADHD and as a matter of fact, the C677T polymorphism is associated with reduced brain volume, especially in white matter regions, potentially indicating a smaller brain size in individuals carrying these mutations; this is often linked to decreased folate metabolism and increased homocysteine levels which may impact brain development and function.

    MTHFR mutations can cause neuroinflammation and that would explain the ADHD behaviors.

    Report comment

    • “High rate,” “associated,” “potentially,” “often linked,” “may impact…”

      All of these are probabalistic statements. There is no clear connection between a particular genetic pattern and all or even most “cases” of “ADHD”. None of these genetic indicators have ever been used to “diagnose” ADHD, mainly because many people with a particular mutation do NOT “have ADHD,” while many who are diagnosed with “ADHD” don’t have the mutation in question.

      Perhaps this begs the larger question: Even if there are genetic associations with SOME cases of “ADHD” that are predictable, why have we decided that these variations in human behavior are a “disorder” or disease condition? Why is there no “Attention excess hypoactivity disorder?” Why is only HYPERactivity considered a problem?

      I think we know the answer. “Hyperactive” children are more difficult for adults to manage, as they don’t (by definition) tend to go along with the program, so we decide that the child is the problem rather than examining our own models of education and other societal expectations. This despite reliable research that so-called “ADHD” children are virtually indistinguishable from “normal” children in open classroom settings, and that “treated” children do not in general have any better outcomes than those who are left to their own devices. This doesn’t even begin to touch the other big issue, namely that abused/neglected children tend to have a much higher rate of “ADHD” diagnoses than the general populace. If it’s all genetic, why is it so much more common in the foster care population?

      I have no doubt that some kids are genetically “programmed” to be more active than others (though this does NOT come close to explaining “cause” for the range of kids so diagnosed). Maybe a range of such people is necessary for success as a species. After all, species survival depends on sufficient genetic diversity. Or as one wise foster kid put it, “Maybe it’s OK for different people to have different brain chemistries.”

      Report comment

  6. SCIENCE DIRECT

    The right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) has a crucial role in inhibitory control in children with ADHD.

    The tDCS effect in children with ADHD in prepotent inhibition is symptom severity- dependent.

    Ongoing inhibition in children with ADHD improves through tDCS independent from symptom severity.

    I am grateful for the proper diagnosis of and treatment for ADHD. I’ve been given a new pair of glasses which has allowed me to engage successfully in a world once beyond my reach. For the past 30 years drugs used to improve concentration opened doors that were locked shut, which led to embarrassment, humiliation, constant failure and disgrace.

    Report comment

  7. There are no known biological causes for any of the psychiatric ‘disorders’, including ‘ADHD’. Consequently, there are no tests available that provide independent, objective data to support any psychiatric ‘diagnosis’ (Council for Evidence-Based Psychiatry, http://www.cepuk.org).

    The central nervous system stimulants prescribed for the ‘diagnosis’ of ‘ADHD’ are as addictive as cocaine (Advocate Children’s Hospital) and have a range of debilitating and dangerous side effects, including:

    – Dizziness
    – Weakness or numbness in an arm or leg
    – Seizures
    – Motor or verbal tics
    – Teeth grinding
    – Depression
    – Delusions
    – Increased suspicion of others
    – Hallucinations
    – Agitation
    – Fever
    – Sweating
    – Confusion
    – Rapid heartbeat
    – Shivering
    – Severe muscle stiffness or twitching
    – Loss of coordination
    – Nausea
    – Vomiting
    – Diarrhea
    – Frenzied or abnormally elevated mood
    – Changes in vision or blurred vision
    – Paleness or blue colouration of fingers or toes
    – Pain, numbness, burning, or tingling in the hands or feet
    – Unexplained wounds on fingers or toes
    – Blistering or peeling skin
    – Swelling of the eyes, face, tongue, or throat
    – Difficulty breathing or swallowing
    – Heart attack
    – Stroke
    – Sudden death (medicineplus.gov)

    Frank, please reflect on the potential harm of your unfounded claims before advocating for the use of unproven ‘therapies’ and harmful drugs for non-existent conditions.

    Report comment

LEAVE A REPLY