Stimulants Don’t Improve Academic Performance in Kids with ADHD

“Efforts to improve learning in children with ADHD should focus on obtaining effective academic instruction rather than stimulant medication.”

8
3606

A new study found that stimulant drugs did not help kids with ADHD perform better on their schoolwork. Although the drugs improved children’s behavior in class, they did not do any better academically when they were taking the drugs than when they were not.

“There was no detectable effect of medication on learning the material taught during instruction,” the researchers write. “Children learned the same amount of subject-area and vocabulary content whether they were taking [Ritalin] or placebo during the instructional period.”

The study, published in the Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, was led by William E. Pelham, Jr. at Florida International University. It also included researchers from the University of California, San Diego; the University of California, Irvine; Florida State University; and Penn State College of Medicine.

The researchers recruited 173 children (between the ages of 7 and 12) who met DSM-5 criteria for ADHD and were involved in a therapeutic summer camp. The study was designed so that one group of kids would receive the stimulant Ritalin (methylphenidate) for three weeks (while the other group received a placebo). Then the groups would switch—with the first group now being put on the placebo and the second group taking Ritalin for three weeks.

With this design, the researchers could compare how the same students did on placebo versus the stimulant drug. In addition, having the two groups switch helps to account for other effects, such as withdrawal effects and the effects of time (such as whether all students were more fidgety earlier—or later—in the program, for instance).

This study is unique because studies of the effects of stimulants don’t assess actual school performance. (The researchers write that this is the first study of its kind.) Instead, studies usually focus on behavior (whether kids are better at sitting still in a classroom, for instance) as rated by teachers or parents. Yet academic performance is one of the critical reasons parents want their kids to take drugs.

The researchers write:

“Although it has been believed for decades that medication effects on academic seatwork productivity and classroom behavior would translate into improved learning of new academic material, we found no such translation.”
They add, “Medication had no detectable impact on how much children learned from academic units of science, social studies, and vocabulary.”

The researchers did find that, when taking the drug, kids were able to complete about one to two more math problems per minute and “committed” about two fewer “rule violations” per hour.

The researchers note that students on the drug also did a tiny bit better on their tests—by 1.7 to 3 points on a 100-point scale. However, they add that this improvement is smaller than the improvement kids see if they get a good night’s sleep before a test.

In conclusion, the researchers write that parents and pediatricians believe that stimulants will lead to academic improvement and that academic achievement is a primary goal for ADHD treatment. The researchers suggest that if this is the goal, stimulants should not be prescribed. Instead, academic instruction and support should be improved:

“Our failure to find an effect of stimulant medication on the learning of individual academic curriculum units raises questions about how stimulant medication would lead to improved academic achievement over time. This is important given that many parents and pediatricians believe that medication will improve academic achievement; parents are more likely to pursue medication (vs. other treatment options) when they identify academic achievement as a primary goal for treatment. The current findings suggest this emphasis may be misguided: Efforts to improve learning in children with ADHD should focus on obtaining effective academic instruction and support (e.g., Individualized Educational Plans) rather than the use of stimulant medication.”

One caveat to this study is that it involved short (25-minute) lessons each day, delivered in a small-group setting, so it is unclear how the results would generalize to a standard, full-length schoolday in a larger classroom. Additionally, the short-term follow-up of the study (three weeks on the drug) makes it unclear if a longer duration of use would be more—or less—effective.

The most well-regarded and highly cited study of childhood ADHD, the NIMH’s MTA study, found promising initial results on ADHD symptoms and behavior, which were published widely in the mass media and led to the belief that stimulants were extremely effective for ADHD treatment. However, later results of the MTA study were dismal: the three-year follow-up found that those receiving treatment were no better off than those who did not, while the six-to-eight-year follow-up found that those who received medication did no better than those who did not. By that follow-up, none of the treatments had been successful: the children who received treatment still scored worse than the normative comparison group on 91% of the measures they tested.

Another recent study found that taking stimulants made kids more than 18 times more likely to experience depression. However, once the kids stopped taking the drug, their risk of depression dropped back down to that of their healthy peers. Similar studies have supported this effect.

 

****

Pelham III, W. E., Altszuler, A. R., Merrill, B. M., Raiker, J. S., Macphee, F. L., Ramos, M., . . . & Pelham Jr, W. E. (2022). The effect of stimulant medication on the learning of academic curricula in children with ADHD: A randomized crossover study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 90(5), 367-380. (Link)

Editor’s Note: The description of the six-to-eight year results of the MTA study was edited for clarity.

8 COMMENTS

  1. I don’t know why this is published as if it were some kind of new news. The first review of stimuants for academic performance was done way back in 1978 by none other than Russell Barkley, the eventual advocate for putting massive numbers of kids on Ritalin. His review showed kids taking stimulants barely outperformed their non-medicated peers, and commented that the difference was so small it was “easily explained by reading the questions more carefully.” Repeated reviews by Swanson (1993) and by the OSU Medication Effectiveness Study in about 2002 or so found no significant improvement for long-term stimulant use in any academic measure. Naturalistic studies like the MTA, the Raine study in Australia, the Quebec study, the long-term MTA results, and a comparison study between Finnish and US children showed again and again that stimulants provide no improvement in any academic measure, and in fact, no improvement in ANY long-term outcome that adults would wish to see improved by “treatment.” This is just one more nail in that coffin, but the sad reality is that psychiatry has not noticed so far that the patient has been dead for decades.

    Report comment

  2. An interesting observation of kids on speed is whether or not their behavior was more appropriate because they thought people, particularly their teachers, were spying on them. I don’t know if “classical” experimenters could find out, but the area’s speed freaks would almost certainly know.

    Report comment

  3. Thank you for your honest reporting on the harm of our children, by the psy-professions and “fierce Pharma,” Peter, et al.

    And I’ll add my sincere request, please stop psychiatric drugging our children … and everyone else, for profit. The psych drugs are neurotoxins. And intentionally poisoning anyone – for any reason – is attempted murder, doctors.

    And you were all taught about anticholinergic toxidrome in med school.

    Report comment

    • Yet oddly, these behavioral changes contributed nothing to long-term outcomes, as multiple studies have shown no academic advantage for taking stimulants over the long term. Kids who took stimulants and kids who did not had very similar academic outcomes, including test scores, HS graduation rates, college enrollment, or even other measures of social outcomes like delinquency scores and self-esteem. So apparently, doing more problems more quickly might get approval in the classroom, but has no actual impact on learning.

      Report comment

LEAVE A REPLY