According to the preprint study, changes in brain development linked to early adversity differ based on age and experience. For example, while poverty is associated with decreased brain volume in certain regions during childhood, these regions begin to have more average volumes as we age. Conversely, early interpersonal adversity (such as maltreatment) is associated with initially greater volumes in some brain regions that reduce as we age. The authors write:
“Findings demonstrate that early-life adversity does not have an ontogenetically uniform impact on brain volumes, but instead exhibits age-, experience-, and region-specific associations. Relative to non-exposed comparisons, interpersonal early adversity (e.g., family-based maltreatment) was associated with initially larger volumes in frontolimbic regions until ~10 years old, after which these exposures were linked to increasingly smaller volumes. By contrast, socioeconomic disadvantage (e.g., poverty) was associated with smaller volumes in temporal-limbic regions in childhood, which were attenuated at older ages.”
The current work examines the link between early life adversity, age, and brain volume. To accomplish this goal, the authors conducted a literature search and identified 92 unique samples from 81 articles with 27,234 participants for inclusion in the meta-analysis. The authors used four inclusion criteria in their search for studies to help them better understand the relationship of early adversity to brain volume:
- Participants aged 18 years and younger.
- A measure of early adversity before the age of 18
- An MRI measurement of brain volume
- Sufficient amount of data to calculate effect size
The authors and their research assistants then coded each of the included articles for early interpersonal adversity (caregiving disruptions, caregiver psychopathology, maltreatment, and interpersonal trauma) and socioeconomic disadvantage (indicators of low family socioeconomic status, non-interpersonal trauma, community crime/violence exposure, neighborhood poverty, and area deprivation). 65% of the included samples (60)Ā involved early interpersonal adversity versus 35% (32) for early socioeconomic disadvantage.
Early interpersonal adversity was associated with a larger amygdala, hippocampus, ventral anterior cingulate cortex, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex until age 12. After age 12, interpersonal adversity was associated with reduced volume in the same regions.
Early socioeconomic disadvantage was associated with a smaller amygdala, hippocampus, parahippocampus, and temporal gyri until the age of 12. However, these differences disappear as participants approach middle and late adolescence.
The authors acknowledge several limitations to the current study. First, the methods used to collect data on adversity in the included articles could not account for possible important factors such as the timing of adversity exposure.
Second, there was not much data for infants and toddlers, likely due to the complications of collecting MRI data from that population.
Third, the data for brain volume were collected cross-sectionally, meaning it came from different participants. A clearer picture of the connection between early adversity and brain volume would likely result if MRI data were collected from the same participant at different ages longitudinally.
Fourth, while articles included in the meta-analysis focused on one type of adversity, it is possible the participants could have been exposed to other types. Finally, 70% of the included samples were from North America, vastly limiting generalizability to other populations.
The authors conclude:
“Findings from this meta-analysis underscore that the careful consideration of development facilitates a deeper appreciation of how the developing brain adapts to specific experience types.”
Recent research has found that poverty leads to a reduced cortical surface area, which is associated with neurological and behavioral problems in adolescents. Children living in poverty show brain physiology similar to adults with frontal lobe damage. Childhood poverty has also been linked to early neurological impairment and smaller brain sizes, whereas financial resources have been found to increase infant brain activity. Authors have also noted that neglect and abuse change children’s brains.
Critics of brain imaging studies have noted that they are likely false due to inflated effect sizes and called such studies “problematic if not unsubstantiated.”Ā One analysis found that MRI studies were unreliable and not suitable for research. At least two studies have found that there are no meaningful brain differences in depressed versus non-depressed populations. Another piece of research found that brain scans cannot differentiate between mental health conditions.
****
Vannucci, A., Fields, A., Hansen, E., Katz, A., Kerwin, J., Tachida, A., Martin, N., & Tottenham, N. (2023). Interpersonal early adversity demonstrates dissimilarity from early socioeconomic disadvantage in the course of Human Brain Development: A meta-analysis. https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.02.16.528877 (Link)
Good to know what these neuroscience departments are up to and the sort of experiments they do, but again, none of this means anything for the average day-to-day individual except a psychologist who reads this will cite it to a patient in “treatment” affected by poverty and childhood maltreatment and keep him in “therapy” rather than give him a lump of money to get him out of that poverty or get him justice from maltreatment.
Report comment
So true.
https://www.madinamerica.com/2016/04/heal-for-life/
https://www.indybay.org/newsitems/2019/01/23/18820633.php?fbclid=IwAR2-cgZPcEvbz7yFqMuUwneIuaqGleGiOzackY4N2sPeVXolwmEga5iKxdo
And one must point out the hypocrisy of psychologists and psychiatrists – none of whom can even bill to help child abuse survivors – to claim they should be the ones who should have the right to help child abuse survivors, or that they are the “experts” on people for whom they can not even bill to help.
https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/your-child-does-not-have-bipolar-disorder/201402/dsm-5-and-child-neglect-and-abuse-1
Report comment
Based on what Iāve read on the MIA website brain imaging studies are not in the least bit accurate in correlating with mental illness. So thatās one reason this meta analysis is dubious.
A second reason for skepticism is the contention that poverty causes mental illness. Iāve seen no proof of this. My immigrant relatives were all poor. They had no more mental illness than the middle and upper middle class people I now know. And, as an aside, Iād bet that the people most hooked on antidepressants are upper middle class women.
Report comment
I think it depends on what you consider āmental illnessā.
Enduring financial hardship and deprivation can cause high levels of stress that can adversely affect peopleās state of mind and ability to function. And unfortunately this usually gets categorized as āmental illnessā.
Report comment
And I wouldnāt be surprised if higher rates of addiction to psychiatric drugs correlates with higher levels of income. After all, thatās just what the pharmaceutical cartels are counting on.
Report comment
Definitely, the doctors do target young moms, whose husbands work in the insurance business, thus have very good insurance.
Report comment
Marie,
My former psychiatrist, who had a brain spec scanner, or whatever it’s called. Even he told me it wasn’t credible enough to bother paying for. So it’s not just MiA who knows those brain imaging machines are invalid, so do likely many of the psychiatrists.
Report comment
Who needs ābrain imagingā to prove that poverty and maltreatment damages children in lasting ways?
Only idiots needs brain scans to realize that emotional scars are as real as physical ones.
Report comment